Solitary Writers Are More Collaborative Than You Think

Solitary Writers Are More Collaborative Than You Think

Psychology February 18, 2012 / By Keith Sawyer
Solitary Writers Are More Collaborative Than You Think
SYNOPSIS

Even famous loners collaborate more than you think.

I just read a fascinating article* by Professor Katherine Giuffre, of Colorado College, that asks the question: Do social networks contribute to creativity? Previous research is pretty compelling: social networks and collaboration contribute to greater creativity. But as Giuffre points out, no one has compared creative and non-creative periods during the lifetime of a single person. As she puts it:

"Over the course of a person's lifetime, there are some moments of creativity and other periods where that same person is not as creative. If we then trace the pattern of social relations over the course of a lifetime, comparing periods of much creative production with periods of no or very little creative production, we have a way of examining the correlation between a person's social relations and his or her creative output (p. 824)."

Emily Dickinson, Paul Gauguin, and Charlotte Bronte were legendary loners, famous for seeking out solitude, far from the cafes and parlors of the big cities. So why were these three creators chosen? Basically, if you could show that even these famous loners benefited from collaboration, you could put the nail in the coffin of the "lone genius" hypothesis:

"Given the contention that creativity is the product not of lonely recluses locked away in their garrets, but of individuals enmeshed in social structures, the most compelling cases to examine will be those of precisely the loneliest of recluses because they are the cases most unfavorable to the hypothesis."

So here's what she did with these three famous creators: She compared their level of correspondence during creative and non-creative periods. For all three of these famous recluses, their letters provide an unusually accurate representation of their social networks, because they all lived and worked primarily alone and rarely had face-to-face creative encounters. Past research suggests that creativity would increase when (1) a person's social network is dense, but not so dense that everyone thinks the same; (2) a person has easy access to a diverse range of other people; (3) a person is linked into many different types of groups. And here's what she found. During periods of high creativity, the density of their social network is about .475, higher than during uncreative periods--meaning that the creator's social network draws together and interacts more frequently. In other words, more collaboration is associated with greater creativity. She concludes:

"It was not when the artists were alone...that they were most creative, but when they were attached to others in a more moderate way and when those others were close to each other, although, again, not so close as to form one cohesive group (p. 836)."

When Giuffre actually read the content of the letters, she found ample confirmation of creative interaction: "The artists actively solicited support and critical feedback for their endeavors from others in their networks" (p. 838). The bottom line: These famous loners were not as isolated as the myths would have it. Communication, collaboration, and social networks contribute to creativity.

*Giuffre, Katherine. (2010). Half the right people: Network density and creativity. Culture Unbound, 2: 819-846.

comments powered by Disqus
RECOMMENDED
FOR YOU